Wednesday, November 23, 2011

THANKS


There are many things for which I am thankful, but one in particular comes to mind.  On December 25, 1940 Anna Bianca Kohnke was born in Hilversum Holland, who later became my wife.  I am thankful, that against considerable odds, she survived the Nazi Holocaust in the Netherlands thanks to the self-sacrifice of her birth parents, Leni and Erich Kohnke (who died in Auschwitz), who turned Anneke over at 18 months of age to a Dutch Underground Resistance courier, Cora de Jong who delivered her to a Dutch family that hid Anneke from 1942-45.  Cora is pictured below at a ceremony in her honor in the Hague in August 2011. 


She was found in early 1946 by Otto Frank, father of Anne Frank, who had been a family friend.  When he found Anneke in a children’s home, he reported that she was standing on a table conducting the other children in singing a song, much like her distinguished musician father had done so many times in Chemnitz, Germany at the Theater Orchestra. 

Thanks to US State Department she was granted an emergency Visa, and due to the generosity of the National Council of Jewish women, her $63.50 fare was paid to sail aboard the USS Marine Flasher, from Le Havre, France to New York in July 6, 1946.  She was met at Pier 97 by her mother’s brother Erich Leyens and sister, Greta Leyens Herzfeld, who served as her parents in New York as she was growing up in Inwood, Washington Heights. 


Sixty five years ago Anneke enjoyed her first Thanksgiving in the US with her Aunt/Mummy Greta.  It must have been a very special Thanksgiving.  Having spent years near starvation at times, it must have seemed something of a miracle to her, surrounded by a feast and people who loved her. 

I am thankful for three daughters, Andrea, Jennifer and Rebecca and a son, Peter, and seven grandchildren and especially to the people who made Anneke's life possible. 

Sunday, November 20, 2011

Lying Down With The Devil: Coordinated Violence Against Unarmed Americans

It has been said, “If you lie down with the devil, you will wake up in hell,” which Hosni Mubarek discovered in Tahrir Square in Cairo and Mummar Gadaffi in Tripoli.  Michael Bloomberg in New York City, Jean Quan in Oakland and Police Chief Annette Spicuzza of the University of California at Davis will discover the same is true for them. 

Beating, pepper spraying, shooting rubber bullets and severely injuring unarmed people who are exercising their First Amendment rights (like Iraq Vet Scott Olsen shown here), will not be tolerated. If those in government think they have problems now, the American people are likely to react with increasing numbers to such intolerable abuses of power, and will take their case to to the streets of Washington DC and the US Capitol.

The widespread use of military force against unarmed citizens in Cairo, Tripoli and Damascus, was condemned by the US Administration and other American political leaders.  Now that the same things are occurring in cities throughout the United States, the Obama Administration has been largely silent about the unprecedented paramilitary violence against Americans exercising their First Amendment rights. 

If Barrack Obama had any hopes of winning the 2012 election with support from most people who voted for him in 2008, he can think again. It has been a display of utter lack of courage. He’d better look to the Tea Party for support.

Mayor Jean Quan of Oakland spilled the beans, that the attacks across 18 US cities are being centrally coordinated by a group with links to the US Department of Homeland Security and paid for by the Motorola Corporation.  Motorola, has close ties to a police organization that is responsible for coordinating the recent crackdown on the Occupy Wall Street protests in various cities.
                      


The organization that is responsible for coordinating these attacks is PERF. PERF stands for Police Executive Research Forum (PERF).  Chuck Wexler (show above) is Executive Director of the Police Executive Research Forum (PERF), a Washington, D.C.-based policing think tank.  He is a consultant and advisory board member of the Department of Homeland Security.
When the mechanisms of democratic government repeatedly fail over a prolonged period, as has occurred in the US, the people take direct action to protest the failure of the elected officials to do their jobs.  The American electorate has been incredibly patient and tolerant of deadlocks and repeated roadblocks by elected officials, to nearly any improvement in the lot of the American people via the Congress and President. 
What did the Koch Brothers and other Right Wing radicals really expect, for the citizens of America to continue to roll over and play dead, in the face of massive abuses occurring in their names carried out by hopelessly corrupt elected officials?

Barrack Obama is a Constitutional scholar.  Apparently he is more of a scholar than elected official, capable of acting on behalf of the American people.  Perhaps he will write an article for a law journal some day about how he turned the country’s first Amendment Rights over to the Motorola Corporation and city's mayors bent on suppressing dissent at any cost. 

Sunday, November 13, 2011

Autism: Visual Memory, Intelligence, and According Respect


Visual Memory and Intelligence: In a series of articles on cognitive functioning in autism, including his most recent Comment in the Nov. 3rd issue of Nature, Dr. Laurent Mottron has emphasized visual perception, and specifically visual pattern recognition and reaction time in autism compared with typical peers. People with autism often do best on subtests that involve no semantic content, i.e. no verbal meaning.  The Raven’s Progressive Mattrices test upon which he bases much of his argument, involves a series of fairly simple geometric shapes and their physical features and spatial orientations and arrangements. 

Some studies suggest that a subset of individuals with autism “solve” these problems by using visuospatial computational operations similar to a computer, and less so on problems requiring verbal reasoning (Kunda, McGreggor and Goel, 2010).  While verbally more capable people solve Raven’s Matrix Problems via verbal strategies, non-verbal people with autism obviously do not.  They rely on memory for spatial configurations.  This appears to be the same configuration memory strategy used in building complex jigsaw puzzles based on patterns of shapes.  The first child I met with autism over 35 years ago, though non-verbal and with very significant cognitive disability, was equally facile at building 500 piece jigsaw puzzles right side up or upside down, and showed no interest in, or recognition of the images created once the puzzle had been constructed.  The process seems to involve an ability to perform extremely complex configural visual memory tasks without any reference to their possible verbal meaning.  Indeed, an attempt to attach semantic meaning may actually detract from such task performance.

It is some stretch of the imagination to call this skill intelligence.  This ability is similar to those used by birds, especially migratory birds.  There is evidence that pigeons can memorize up to 725 different visual patterns such as might be encountered in a stream of such patterns while in migratory flight.  The visual patterns are extremely complex and often involve entirely irregular patterns with forested areas, lakes, farmland, streams, towns and so on, in entirely unpredictable patterns, constantly changing as the bird travels, sometimes up to 1,800 miles.  It appears that if some birds are blown off course, they fly circling backward until they reach a familiar visual pattern, and then resume migration.
Other data indicate that fine discriminations between various colors and achromatic patterns can be reliably detected in quails using the brainstem-thalamus complex, in other words, not even involving higher brain areas, such as the visual cortex.  Laboratory studies with pigeons indicate birds are capable of detecting letters (black figures against a white background) with amazing speed, and only exhibiting longer latencies when the correct target stimulus is very infrequent.  Response latencies are typically extremely short, under one second.  It doesn’t appear this ability has anything to do with intelligence.

In reviewing the evidence for “avian intelligence,” which is something of a misnomer, crows, ravens, jackaws, jays and magpies and parrots appear to be capable of more complex problem solving than other birds and in many cases even apes (Emery, 2006). They are even known to use tools.  Despite their extraordinary visual processing abilities, migratory birds generally do not possess these cognitive problem-solving abilities.

It is not my purpose at all to diminish the importance of unique cognitive abilities of many individuals with autism, but to suggest that the ability to perform complex visual configural search tasks with short latencies, as is involved in Mottron’s and his colleagues laboratory and clinical testing tasks, likely has a limited amount to do with what is usually meant by intelligence.  Intelligence is distinguished by ability to use abstract symbols for functional communication in attaching meaning to one’s experiences and in solving problems.  Most people with autism display such skills, some to extraordinary degrees.  Visual memory for configurations is not central to intelligence, though it is indeed a unique skill, very beneficial in the visual arts, as shown here with Stephen Wiltshire, and mathematics and computer science. 
Superiority?

Mottron in his Nature article posed the possibility people with autism are actually “superior” to other people in certain cognitive intellectual skills.  This is a very strange conclusion for a scientist to draw.  In conducting research, scientists don’t conclude that because one group scores higher or lower than another on some measure, that they are “superior” or “inferior.”  I doubt any scientist studying height would say men in Sweden are “superior” to men in India because on average they are taller.  “Superiority” is not within scientific lexicon.
Respect

Moreover, as I pointed out recently on my website, Autism Treatment, to suggest that any group of people must meet some measurable criterion on a test, like shorter latencies on a visual search task, in order to be accorded respect by the rest of society and our world community is inherently offensive. People with autism deserve equal treatment and rights as everyone else because they are members of the human family.  They do not need to prove themselves. “When you are content to be simply yourself and don't compare or compete, everybody will respect you,” Lao Tzu.

If people with autism have difficulties with employment, or socially succeeding in college, perhaps it has to do with social and communication challenges, not intelligence.  By working with experienced speech/language pathologists, psychologists, behavior analysts, vocational experts and social workers, most people with autism spectrum disorders can overcome these impediments to full participation.  Much as a medical student needs training to become a doctor or an architect needs training and practice to be an effective architect, some people on the autism spectrum profit from special training to master some complex social and communication skills required to be an effective teacher, a sales manager or physician. 

References


Emery, NJ. Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. . 361:(1465) 23-43.

Kabai, P, and Kovach, JK (2010) Unconditional discrimination as a paradigm for investigating visual processing in the Avian brainstem. Acta Biol. Hung. 48 (1): 9-14

Mottron, L. (2001) The power of autism. Nature (Comment); 497: 33-35.

Bingman, VP and Able, KP (2002) Maps in birds: representational mechanisms and neural bases. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 12: 745-750

Ritchison, G. (2011) Pigeons can memorize up to 725 different visual patterns. Personal Communication. Professor of Biological Sciences. Eastern Kentucky University

Soulie`res, I,  Michelle Dawson, M. et.al. and Mottron, L. (2009) Enhanced Visual Processing Contributes to Matrix Reasoning in Autism. Human Brain Mapping 30:4082–4107
Thompson, T. (2011) More Mischief: Autism Superiority? Autism Treatment, http://www.travisithompson.net> Nov. 5. 2011.  Accessed Nov. 11, 2011

Tuesday, November 8, 2011

SENSORY PRETEND* DISORDER


You’ve heard them all… sensory overload, sensory integration dysfunction, auditory hyperacuity, sensory processing disorder, and the “out of sync” child, are all ubiquitous terms in the autism field.   Every parent or teacher of children with autism spectrum disorder knows their child or student reacts in usual ways, often very negatively, to some sounds, and they tend to visually fixate on moving objects, flickering lights or vertical lines, such as waving lines (like wiggling a pencil back and forth in front of their eyes).

Some children cover their ears or eyes and wave their head back and forth in the presence of certain sounds.  Nearly without exception, parents of children with autism tell me their child has  sensory integration” problems, which is presumably what they have been told at the time of their child’s diagnostic assessment was conducted, or school team special education evaluation meeting was held.  Parents didn’t spontaneously come up with the notion of a “sensory integration” deficit or “sensory processing disorder” on their own, though they all correctly note that their child often reacts negatively to loud sounds, sirens, bright lights or sometimes textures or smells.   Until Jean Ayres (shown below) book was published in 1970 (Sensory Integration and the Child), while unusual responses to some stimuli were widely recognized among parents and professionals, children with autism were not known to have a sensory integration disorder.  She invented that idea out of whole cloth.  Let’s take a step back for a moment.

Motor Therapy:  There is strong evidence that specific Occupational Therapy activities designed to improve fine and gross motor skills can be very helpful to many children with autism, such as grasping a spoon or pencil, printing or writing, buttoning, lacing and tying shoes, pressing buttons on a phone or computer balancing on one foot, walking up and down stairs, running, throwing and catching, and so on. Motor Apraxia is common in autism Motor apraxia is common among younger children as compared with older children motor apraxia (34%) (Ming et.al. 2007; Dowell et.al. 2009; Dziuk, et.al. 2007). A special set of helpful OT skills are involved in treating Oral-Motor Apraxia, coordinating the movements in the mouth, such as tongue, breathing and swallowing, permitting a child to chew and swallow food and to speak (Hayden and Square, 1994; Law et.al, 2004).  OT’s often refer to them as Praxis or Motor Planning activities. Praxis is a Greek word used to describe the learned ability to plan and to carry out sequences of coordinated movements in order to achieve an objective. Such skills require repeated practice coordinating responses with kinesthetic feedback from having made those responses in proper coordinated sequence.  I say this because I don’t want to create the impression that I am opposed to Occupational Therapy as a profession, which can have a great deal to contribute to improving practical functioning of many children with autism.  We have had excellent experience combining OT-suggested motor coordination interventions with behavioral intervention strategies to gradually promote persistence and enjoyment of motor skills development activities.  The comments in this the remainder of article are specifically about the notion of Sensory Processing Disorder, not overcoming dyspraxia, which is a well established neurological medical condition.
What is a Disorder? A disorder is a functional abnormality or disturbance. Medical disorders can be categorized into mental, physical, genetic, emotional and behavioral disorders. The term disorder is used primarily to identify abnormal conditions that are not caused by infectious organisms such a bacteria or viruses.  Disorders often have known physical causes (e.g. metabolic disorders or epileptic disorder), characteristic measurable features, epidemiology within a population, a characteristic course and established treatment and prognosis.  Just because a group of people deviate from a norm doesn’t mean they have a disorder.  For example, most people from Viet Nam are shorter on average than most other people throughout the World, but that doesn’t mean they have a disorder.  A set of symptoms deviating from the norm can be caused by another condition, but are not themselves a disorder.  The fact that some people lose consciousness, and exhibit abnormalities of muscle tone with violent contractions and relaxation of muscles, is in itself not a disorder, but epilepsy is a disorder that causes those symptoms.  Neurologists don’t attempt to treat the violent contractions, except in emergencies, but they treat the physiological or physical brain abnormalities giving rise to the seizures, using medication or surgery.  If they administer a drug to interrupt a series of seizures, once the person has recovered, that has no effect on the underlying epileptic disorder. 

Sensory Integration or Processing Disorder?  Now that we have a definition of “disorder,” let’s try to make sense of the claim that a child has a “sensory processing,” or “sensory integration” disorder, which can be treated using sensory integration therapy methods.  To do so, we need to consider each of five distinct issues, which often get muddled when trying to sort this out.

1. The first, “Is it true that most, if not all children with autism spectrum disorders react in unusual ways to various kinds of sensory input?” The answer is clearly yes.  Moreover, studies by Winifred Dunn and colleagues at the University of Kansas Medical Center with her Sensory Profile assessment has made it possible to describe in some detail the specific kinds of stimuli and types of responses a given child exhibits under an array of circumstances (Dunn et.al, 1997).  But assuming these differences in responding to stimuli implies the child has a specific, single, tangible, treatable disorder or disability that is causally related to the autism syndrome has not been shown. Not at all.  That would be like concluding that treating the hearing problems of many children with Fetal Alcohol Syndrome would eliminate their underlying FAS.  Obviously, it doesn’t.  It can be very important to fit a hearing impaired child with a hearing aid, but that has no effect on his underlying FAS, which includes cognitive, behavioral challenges and often heart defects, and no one expects it to have such an effect.

An older study found hypersensitivity to sounds was present in about 18% of children with autism but not their typical peers, but by no means ALL children with autism (Khalfa S, J et.al. 1999). A more recent study conducted in France suggests children with autism tend to hear a narrower range of sound frequencies but perceive sounds they do hear, as being louder than same age typical peers (Hear Res. 2004,198:87-92). A study by Tharpe and colleagues at Vanderbilt University indicated that all physiological measures of auditory thresholds were similar among kids with autism and their matched peers, but tests requiring behavioral cooperation of the children with autism revealed less sensitivity and more variability to sounds (Tharpe. et.al., 2006).  Probably has more to do with the required cooperation than underlying auditory processing problem.  In summary, there is some inconsistent evidence suggesting that some measures have indicated some sounds may appear louder to children with autism, but not all.

2. The second question, is whether there are physiological brain differences associated with this tendency to overreact to some stimuli (e.g. sirens), and under-react to others (e.g. people’s speech).  The answer seems to be that while there are some physiological differences, they are unpredictable and don’t clearly relate to the observed exaggerated behavioral reactions to strange sounds, tastes or textures, such as tantrums or self-injury.  There is no obvious link between sound thresholds and tantrums.  The above Tharpe study at the Wilkerson Speech and Hearing Center at Vanderbilt University to test this idea.  Pure tones were presented to children in a classroom setting.  An observer didn’t know when the tones were being sounded but simply recorded any signs of child distress, aggression, tantrums or self-stimulation.  There was no association between presentation of the tones and any of these behavioral symptoms.

3. Thirdly, is there physical evidence that there is something about the way a child’s nervous system functions that indicates that a phenomenon of the alleged “sensory processing disorder,” or  “sensory hyperacuity” actually exists as a measurable brain dysfunction. The answer is no, no such evidence exists.  This notion is based on one of the oldest fallacies in human history, namely reification.  The English word comes from the German word Verdinglichung, which refers to concluding an abstract idea or phenomen has living existence with its own independent properties, by giving such an abstract idea a name, or thingification, treating it as if it exists as a real, honest to God, tangible, touchable, manipulable thing.  In other words, now that a child’s over-responsiveness to some stimuli has been assigned a name, "Sensory Processing Disorder", it must therefore exist as a real thing, which is fallacious. All we know is that the child reacts oddly to some sounds some of the time under some circumstances. Those reactions could be epiphenomenal, i.e. due to something else about autism that causes over reactions to some stimuli.  I strongly support research to identify the causes (they are probably multiple) for these exaggerated responses to some stimuli, but seriously question the validity of the notion of Sensory Processing Disorder. 

4. The last and most challenging problem associated with alleged Sensory Processing Disorder, is whether having identified a child as, “Having a set of unusual sensory characteristics, is there some specific thing we can do to reduce that tendency.  In other words is there an objectively specifiable intervention that will overcome the condition?”  A corollary is “Having conducted such treatment, are the child’s core autism characteristics reduced or eliminated?”  The answer to both questions is no, there is no such sensory integration intervention that has lasting effects on the core features of autism, namely communication deficit, lack of social understanding and social skills and persistent, repetitive non-functional routines. I invite you to conduct your own literature search at PubMed or PsychLit. You will find no compelling evidence for effective treatments. Considering that Jean Ayres published her book over 40 years ago, that suggests perhaps no such evidence will ever be forthcoming. 

So What Do We Make of This?  In her book, Sensory Integration and the Child (1970), Jean Ayres presented theoretical argument that children with autism fail to appropriately integrate sensory input from multiple modalities, which led to the creation of the field of Sensory Integration in occupational therapy, which is used throughout the world despite lack of clear empirical evidence that it produces lasting effects on the communicative, social or other behaviour of children with autism. According to Group Dynamics in Occupational Therapy (2005), Ayres' theory and conclusions remain controversial.  A 2002 article in The Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice reported that controlled studies examining the effect of Sensory Integration treatment "have found little support for the efficacy of SIT for treating children with various developmental disabilities". Controversial Therapies for Developmental Disabilities (2004) indicated that in spite of "limited and mostly negative research findings on SIT", the sensory integration approach remained popular among occupational therapists and "other professionals and clinical populations."  Over a decade ago, Dawson and Watling (2000) reviewed evidence concerning occupational therapy methods and found at that time there were no controlled studies to review. The following year, the stellar panel at the National Research Council came to the same conclusion (Lord & McGee, 2001). They found several studies using Sensory Integration Therapy specifically and concluded, “Five studies of auditory integration training were found. Results of these studies provided no, or at best equivocal, support for the use of auditory integration training in autism.”  The American Speech-Language Hearing Association Working Group on Auditory Integration training issued a report on AIT in 2004, http://www.asha.org/docs/html/TR2004-00260.html concluding that of the six studies testing AIT in autism, none present evidence the method is effective in reducing hyper-responsiveness to sounds.

Why Caregivers are Attracted to Sensory Integration:  Parents and schools are attracted to sensory integration activities because they can see something very tangible occurring before their eyes, which is being done to, and with the child, by a caring, helpful adult, such as an OT, teacher or parent. And what is being done is often very emotionally appealing (e.g. such as holding or swinging a child) and often fun for the adults.  Many children seem to enjoy some SIT activities, but not all.  Some kids balk and scream at being held tightly or brushed or presented with pulsating lights or sounds.  Adults usually reinforce the tantrum by stopping their brushing or hugging the child. 

Parents often take pleasure in conducting the SIT activities on their own, which provides a professionally-sanctioned activity which they are able to do with their own child, which is often difficult otherwise. It makes parents feel they are actually doing something to help their child.  It is fun to blow bubbles with your child, or massage her or wrap him in blanket and hold him in a hug, or run water through his fingers as he giggles.  What can be wrong with that?  Nothing, but don’t expect it to have any effect on your child’s core autism symptoms.  You need to understand you are doing this because it is an enjoyable activity to do with your child, but it is very unlikely to produce any long-term benefits.  

In short, it is really a misnomer to call it therapy, meaning a treatment that produces improvement in a condition or disorder. By all means do things with your child that s/he and you enjoy, but don’t confuse it with treatment designed to produce lasting improvements in their social and communication skills and reduce their rigid, repetitive non-functional routines.

Sensory Diets, Pretend Therapy: A very popular idea with parents and some teachers, is the use of Sensory Rooms and a “sensory diet.”  One company in particular has promoted this notion, the SNOEZELEN company owned by ROMPA in Chesterfield
Derbyshire, UK. They say on their website that, “Children and adults with disabilities or other limiting conditions (who doesn’t have some kind of “limiting condition?) enjoy gentle stimulation of the primary senses...Participants experience self-control, autonomous discovery, and exploration-achievements that overcome inhibitions, enhance self-esteem, and reduce tension. Free from the expectations of others and away from the pressures of directed care, they recuperate (recuperate from what?) and relax. Research has shown that multisensory environments offer a wealth of benefits, often affording the participant and caregiver an opportunity to improve communications, enhance their understanding of each other, and build trust in their relationship.”  I have done considerable literature research attempting to find any empirical evidence of such benefits and have found none. Zero, nada, as in none. There are testimonials galore, but no research.   I have found no measures of self-esteem, improved communication or understanding among children and adults, and certainly no evidence of recuperation, as claimed resulting from the use of Sensory Diet.  Nor have I found any evidence of lasting reductions in compulsive repetitive behavior or increases in social skills or communication associated with use of Sensory Rooms. None whatsoever. 


If one does a search of what is involved in setting up a “Sensory Room,” several suggested lists of supplies and equipment can readily be found on the internet.  Here are just a few examples: LED lights strips, 54 inch bubble tower, laser show machines, liquid light projector, Black Lights and accessories, Mathmos Lights, Fiber Optic Sphere, Beanbag Chairs, Sheepskin Rug, Mini-Bubble Wall, guided imagery tapes, relaxation music, nature sound machines, heartbeat sound stuffed animals or blankets, metronomes, vibrating teethers, blow toys, musical instruments, rubber tubing, ‘Nuk” toothbrush, “Blow Darts,” kazoos, “Z-vibes,” “Jigglers,” tactile toys and balls, textured walls,  vibrating toys, ball pits, “Somatron” vibrating mats and cloud chairs, and the lists go on and on and on.  The photos of “sensory rooms,” available on the internet suggest they were designed by adults who grew up in the1960s in the heyday of psychedelic light shows, for those old enough to remember the Jefferson Airplane at the Filmore Auditorium or the Jimi Hendrix Experience at the CNE Auditorium, though the sound level is obviously much lower.  Sure must be a lot of fun for the parents, OTs and teachers, and hopefully doesn’t terrify the kids too much. Some of it is likely to provoke self-stimulation among some kids, but what the heck, while it doesn’t do any good, it’s not the end of the world.  On the other hand, I have considerable reservations about the appropriateness of parents using Federal Medicaid Waiver money for such purposes since there is no evidence of benefit. 
Summary:  
Lack of Empirical Evidence: There is evidence many children with autism react negatively in exaggerated ways to some stimuli, most often sounds.  In addition, they tend to become narrowly focused on some intermittent visual stimuli and incorporate them into self-stimulatory behavior patterns, while being unresponsive to others, such as spoken speech.  There is some evidence of differences in physiological auditory measures involving the child’s cooperating with the examiner.  Physiological measures that don’t rely on cooperation with the audiologist, generally show few if any auditory functioning differences among children with autism and typical peers. There is little or no evidence from controlled studies that Sensory Integration Training produces any lasing effects on core autism symptoms, though several studies have suggested transient reductions in stereotypic activities, such as rocking or hand-flapping for up to a half hour immediately following SIT activities. 
History Repeats Itself: Many professionals and parents have bought into Jean Ayre’s “Sensory Integration” theory, and are having great difficulty retreating from their stated commitment in the face of a large body of evidence indicating this is not a meaningful term for an actual condition that can be effectively treated as proposed.  This situation is very reminiscent of what happened in psychiatry in the 60s when much of psychiatry was dominated by psychoanalysis, which was largely abandoned by the 80s, along with Bruno Bettleheim’s damaging ideas about the alleged maternal cause of autism.  Many psychiatrists continued to cling to Freudian psychoanalytic theory and treatments long after everyone else in the mental health and psychopathology fields realized it was a misguided approach lacking empirical evidence.  Eventually health insurance companies stopped paying for psychoanalytic therapy, at which point practical exigencies led most psychoanalytic therapists to either retire or switch to other types of treatment.
Costs of Ineffective Services: Most children with autism in the US receive occupational therapy services that incorporate sensory integration training, either in school or privately, the latter usually paid for by medical insurance.  Private OTs generally charge $100 per hour for Sensory Integration Therapy through private agencies.  School-based OTs are paid $61,590 to $75,260 per year or about $30-36 per hour.  Over 300,000 children with autism receive special education services through public schools (CDC, 2006), or conservatively $9 to $10.8 million per year (minimum) plus the cost of private Sensory Integration Therapy at three-times the cost.   If you run the numbers, you can see that a sizable amount of money is being expended for Sensory Integration Therapy for which there is little evidence of benefit.  Substantial money is at stake for the therapists and the general public in terms of the cost of health care insurance. 

I haven’t tried to calculate how much it costs to create a “Sensory Room,” and provide individualized “sensory diets,” for children with autism.  The cost could be very substantial to individual families or school programs, in some cases thousands of dollars per family or school, and there is no evidence whatsoever that it is beneficial.  It may be great fun for the caregiving adults, but of no real benefit to their children with autism. Parents obviously enjoy creating a room full of weird pulsating pink, blue and green lights, use of UV dark-lights, and fur rugs, vibrating mats and cushy squeeze toys, with pulsating heart beat or nature sounds.  

I know, I’m a wet blanket at the party.  I can just hear a parent who recently spent a bundle constructing a “Sensory Room,” in their home saying, “You’re no fun…. You’re a party-pooper.”  I confess, I don’t subscribe to channeling dead relatives, faith healing or Sensory Diets, as much fun as those activities may be for some people, and very lucrative for others. 

There is no empirical evidence for any of them.  Couching an intervention in the language of science does not make it scientific.  Phrenology referred to treating people based on bumps on the skull, which surely must have meant it was scientific. No, not really.  It was a made up theory of Franz Joseph Gall, without any scientific basis.  Sensory Integrative Dysfunction… Sensory Integration Therapy… Sensory Diets?  Same deal.  Made up stuff with no convincing scientific evidence that this is an actual, treatable condition.  Misleading parents into believing they are helping their children overcome their core autism symptoms, when there is little credible evidence to support that assertion, raises ethical questions.  

Fun is really important for everyone.  Eating a hotdog and drinking a coke with your family while watching a baseball game on a sunny July afternoon can be really fun, but it isn’t therapy.  Fun is especially important to kids with autism when it involves shared experiences with their parents, but it isn’t usually the same as therapy or treatment. Treatment, which can also be fun at times, such as Pragmatic Speech Therapy, or Oral-Motor Apraxia therapy, Intensive Early Behavioral Intervention alone or combined with other approaches such as the Denver Early Start Model and LEAP, produce long term improvements in core autism symptoms enabling most kids to function reasonably well in school, home and the community.  If you want to have fun with your child with an ASD that is more likely to have lasting beneficial effects, collaborate with your child’s therapist, speech/language clinician or special ed. teacher to prepare her or him to go camping, or go on a family picnic, or a ball game, a trip to the zoo, or to a county fair and check out the hay bailers and the cows, and eat an ice cream cones… do cool stuff (see Thompson, 2008), but don’t fool yourself into believing flashing LED lights, soft cushy rugs, a hammock or massaging are going to cure your child of her or his autism, or for that matter, even significantly reduce his or her symptoms.

References

American Speech-Language Hearing Association Working Group on Auditory Integration training. http://www.asha.org/docs/html/TR2004-00260.html 

Ayres, A. Jean (1970). Sensory Integration and the Child. Western Psychological Services.

Brown, NB and Dunn, W. (2010) Relationship between context and sensory processing in children with autism. Am J Occup Ther. 2010 May-Jun;64(3):474-83.

Center for Disease Control (2006) Mental Health in the United States: Parental Report of Diagnosed Autism in Children Aged 4--17 Years --- United States, 2003--2004. MMWR Weekly, May 5, 2006: 55(17) 481-86.

Dawson, G and Watling, R. (2000) Interventions to facilitate auditory, visual, and motor integration in autism: A review of the evidence. J Autism Dev Disord. 30(5):415-21.

Dowell, LR, Mahone, EM and Mostofsky, SH (2009) Associations of postural knowledge and basic motor skill with dyspraxia in autism: implication for abnormalities in distributed connectivity and motor learning. Neuropsychology 23 (5) 563-70

Dziuk, MA et. Al. (2007) Dyspraxia in autism: association with motor, social, and communicative deficits. Dev. Med Child Neurol. 49(10) 734-9.

Dunn, W and Westman, K. (1997) The sensory profile: the performance of a national sample of children without disabilities. Am J Occup Ther. 51(1):25-34.

Hayden, D., & Square, P. (1994). Motor speech treatment hierarchy: A systems approach. Clinics in Communication Disorders, 4, 162 – 174.

Herbert, JD, Sharp,
IR, and Gaudiano, BA (2002) Separating Fact from Fiction in the Etiology and Treatment of Autism: A Scientific Review of the Evidence. Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice. Spring ~ Summer 2002 Volume 1 Number 1. http://www.srmhp.org/0101/autism.html

Jacobson, JW, Foxx, RM and Mulick, JA (2004) Controversial therapies for developmental disabilities: Fad, Fasion and Science in Professional Practice. Lawrence Erlbaum, Associates.

Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2004). The efficacy of treatment for children with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing Research, 47, 4, 924-943.

Lord, CE and McGee, J. (eds) (2001) Educating Children with Autism. Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Academy Press, pages 98-99.

Ming, X, Brimacombe, M and Wagner, GC (200) Prevalence of motor impairment in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Dev. 29 (9) 565-79.

NINDS Developmental Dyspraxia Information Page; http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/dyspraxia/dyspraxia.htm  Accessed 11-8-2001

Rosenhall U, Nordin V, Sandström M, Ahlsén G, Gillberg C. (1999) Autism and hearing loss. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999 Oct;29(5):349-57.

Stephenson, J and Carter, M. (2009) The use of weighted vests with children with autism and other disabilities.  J Autism Dev Disord. 39(1):105-14.

Tharpe, AM et. Al. (2006) Auditory characteristics of children with autism. Ear Hear.;27(4):430-41.

Thompson, T. (2008) Dr. Thompson’s Straight Talk on Autism.  Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes, Inc.  (Chapter 10. The importance of leisure, p. 215).
--------------------------
*The very popular Peabody award-winning children’s radio show Let’s Pretend, which began broadcasting in 1934 on Saturday mornings, had an audience full of children beginning with Uncle Bill Adams' salutation, "Hello Pretenders!" followed by the response "Hello, Uncle Bill!"  The show presented adaptations of classic children's stories and fairy tales like Rumpelstiltskin, Sleeping Beauty, Thumbelina and many, many more.  Everyone understand they were “pretend” stories, unlike the pretend stories about Sensory Processing Disorder, which regrettably too many people take seriously.